TY - JOUR
T1 - The role of answer fluency and perceptual fluency in the monitoring and control of reasoning
T2 - Reply to Alter, Oppenheimer, and Epley (2013)
AU - Thompson, Valerie A.
AU - Ackerman, Rakefet
AU - Sidi, Yael
AU - Ball, Linden J.
AU - Pennycook, Gordon
AU - Prowse Turner, Jamie A.
PY - 2013/8
Y1 - 2013/8
N2 - In this reply, we provide an analysis of Alter et al. (2013) response to our earlier paper (Thompson et al., 2013). In that paper, we reported difficulty in replicating Alter, Oppenheimer, Epley, and Eyre's (2007) main finding, namely that a sense of disfluency produced by making stimuli difficult to perceive, increased accuracy on a variety of reasoning tasks. Alter, Oppenheimer, and Epley (2013) argue that we misunderstood the meaning of accuracy on these tasks, a claim that we reject. We argue and provide evidence that the tasks were not too difficult for our populations (such that no amount of "metacognitive unease" would promote correct responding) and point out that in many cases performance on our tasks was well above chance or on a par with Alter et al.'s (2007) participants. Finally, we reiterate our claim that the distinction between answer fluency (the ease with which an answer comes to mind) and perceptual fluency (the ease with which a problem can be read) is genuine, and argue that Thompson et al. (2013) provided evidence that these are distinct factors that have different downstream effects on cognitive processes.
AB - In this reply, we provide an analysis of Alter et al. (2013) response to our earlier paper (Thompson et al., 2013). In that paper, we reported difficulty in replicating Alter, Oppenheimer, Epley, and Eyre's (2007) main finding, namely that a sense of disfluency produced by making stimuli difficult to perceive, increased accuracy on a variety of reasoning tasks. Alter, Oppenheimer, and Epley (2013) argue that we misunderstood the meaning of accuracy on these tasks, a claim that we reject. We argue and provide evidence that the tasks were not too difficult for our populations (such that no amount of "metacognitive unease" would promote correct responding) and point out that in many cases performance on our tasks was well above chance or on a par with Alter et al.'s (2007) participants. Finally, we reiterate our claim that the distinction between answer fluency (the ease with which an answer comes to mind) and perceptual fluency (the ease with which a problem can be read) is genuine, and argue that Thompson et al. (2013) provided evidence that these are distinct factors that have different downstream effects on cognitive processes.
KW - Analytic thinking
KW - Answer fluency
KW - Dual process theories
KW - Intuition
KW - Metacognition
KW - Perceptual fluency
UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/record.url?scp=84892487454&partnerID=8YFLogxK
U2 - 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.03.003
DO - 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.03.003
M3 - ???researchoutput.researchoutputtypes.contributiontojournal.comment???
C2 - 23571071
AN - SCOPUS:84892487454
SN - 0010-0277
VL - 128
SP - 256
EP - 258
JO - Cognition
JF - Cognition
IS - 2
ER -